苗秀影 遲立忠
(北京體育大學(xué)運(yùn)動(dòng)人體科學(xué)學(xué)院運(yùn)動(dòng)心理學(xué)教研室, 北京 100084)
調(diào)節(jié)定向理論(regulatory focus)認(rèn)為, 個(gè)體具有促進(jìn)定向(promotion focus)和防御定向(prevention focus)兩種定向方式。促進(jìn)定向的個(gè)體更渴望“獲得成功”, 在目標(biāo)達(dá)成的過程中多采用較為積極的行為策略; 防御定向的個(gè)體更傾向于“避免失敗”, 在目標(biāo)達(dá)成的過程中多采用更為保守的行為策略(Fuglestad, Rothman, & Jeffery, 2013;Higgins, 2000; Higgins, Idson, Freitas, Spiegel, &Molden, 2003; Kao, 2012; Keller & Bless, 2006;Scott, Sereno, & O'Donnell, 2012; Worthy, Maddox,& Markman, 2007)。當(dāng)不同調(diào)節(jié)定向的個(gè)體采用符合自身定向方式的策略時(shí), 就達(dá)成了調(diào)節(jié)匹配(regulatory fit)。處于匹配狀態(tài)時(shí), 個(gè)體的行為表現(xiàn)會(huì)更好并且更愿意堅(jiān)持與自身定向方式一致的行為策略, 此即調(diào)節(jié)匹配效應(yīng)(段錦云, 周冉, 陸文娟, 李晶, 朱宜超, 2013; Chatterjee, Roy, &Malshe, 2011; Park, van Dyne, & Ilgen, 2013; Vaughn,2010)。
通過梳理運(yùn)動(dòng)領(lǐng)域的相關(guān)研究發(fā)現(xiàn), 調(diào)節(jié)匹配對(duì)運(yùn)動(dòng)表現(xiàn)的提高、運(yùn)動(dòng)決策的改善以及鍛煉行為的維持均具有積極的影響, 運(yùn)動(dòng)領(lǐng)域中對(duì)調(diào)節(jié)匹配的研究顯示出了較好的理論與應(yīng)用價(jià)值(Daryanto, de Ruyter, Wetzels, & Patterson, 2010;Debanne, Angel, & Fontayne, 2014; Plessner,Unkelbach, Memmert, Baltes, & Kolb, 2009)。教練員和運(yùn)動(dòng)員可以參照調(diào)節(jié)匹配理論合理地選擇比賽策略, 例如對(duì)于促進(jìn)定向的運(yùn)動(dòng)員, 可通過激發(fā)其“獲勝動(dòng)機(jī)”, 使其主動(dòng)應(yīng)對(duì)逆境從而有效地完成比賽; 而對(duì)于防御定向的運(yùn)動(dòng)員, 可通過引導(dǎo)他們關(guān)注積極可控的方面使其在比賽中正常發(fā)揮。
有研究者將調(diào)節(jié)匹配的實(shí)驗(yàn)操控方式分為“整 合 式 ” (integral manipulation)和 “獨(dú) 立 式 ”(incidental manipulation)?!罢鲜健辈倏v主要通過操作任務(wù)定向以匹配不同調(diào)節(jié)定向的個(gè)體(特質(zhì)性調(diào)節(jié)定向), 這種匹配整合于當(dāng)前的任務(wù)或情境,能對(duì)當(dāng)前的任務(wù)產(chǎn)生直接、即時(shí)的影響?!蔼?dú)立式”操縱主要通過啟動(dòng)個(gè)體的定向方式(狀態(tài)性調(diào)節(jié)定向)以匹配任務(wù)定向, 這種匹配獨(dú)立于當(dāng)前的任務(wù)或情境, 能對(duì)隨后的任務(wù)產(chǎn)生間接、延續(xù)的影響(王懷勇, 2011; Righetti, Francesca, & Rusbult,2011; Vaughn, Harkness, & Clark, 2010)。運(yùn)動(dòng)領(lǐng)域研究多采用“整合式”對(duì)調(diào)節(jié)匹配進(jìn)行操控, 即主要通過操控任務(wù)定向使之與運(yùn)動(dòng)員的調(diào)節(jié)定向構(gòu)成匹配狀態(tài)進(jìn)而考察其對(duì)當(dāng)前任務(wù)或情境的影響。例如, 通過指導(dǎo)語(yǔ)“(高爾夫球在)距離球洞 2米的位置, 你有5次擊球機(jī)會(huì), 你需要至少擊中3次”構(gòu)建獲得性(eagerness approach)任務(wù)定向; 通過指導(dǎo)語(yǔ)“(高爾夫球在)距離球洞 2米的位置, 你有5次擊球機(jī)會(huì), 你最多可以失誤2次”構(gòu)建規(guī)避性(vigilance avoidance)任務(wù)定向(Kutzner, F?rderer,& Plessner, 2013)。當(dāng)調(diào)節(jié)定向(通過調(diào)節(jié)定向問卷(regulatory focus questionnaire)進(jìn)行測(cè)量)與任務(wù)定向一致時(shí)為匹配狀態(tài)(即促進(jìn)定向——獲得性任務(wù)定向、防御定向——規(guī)避性任務(wù)定向), 反之為不匹配狀態(tài)(即促進(jìn)定向——規(guī)避性任務(wù)定向、防御定向——獲得性任務(wù)定向)。其中, 促進(jìn)定向與獲得性任務(wù)定向相匹配時(shí)稱為促進(jìn)匹配, 防御定向與規(guī)避性任務(wù)定向匹配時(shí)稱為防御匹配(Barber & Mather, 2013; Chen, 2014; Ludolph &Schulz, 2015; Scholer, Ozaki, & Higgins, 2014)。
目前運(yùn)動(dòng)表現(xiàn)中調(diào)節(jié)匹配效應(yīng)的研究主要集中在球類項(xiàng)目上(如足球、籃球), 主要考察調(diào)節(jié)匹配對(duì)運(yùn)動(dòng)員進(jìn)球成功率的影響(Memmert et al.,2009; Plessner et al., 2009)。有研究者考察了調(diào)節(jié)匹配對(duì)足球運(yùn)動(dòng)員點(diǎn)球成功率的影響, 結(jié)果顯示,當(dāng)處于匹配狀態(tài)時(shí)(即告知促進(jìn)定向的運(yùn)動(dòng)員“5次罰球中至少要踢進(jìn) 3次”或告知防御定向的運(yùn)動(dòng)員“5次罰球中最多可以有 2次失誤”), 運(yùn)動(dòng)員的點(diǎn)球成功率高于調(diào)節(jié)不匹配時(shí)(即告知促進(jìn)定向運(yùn)動(dòng)員“5次罰球中最多可以有2次失誤”、或告知防御定向運(yùn)動(dòng)員“5次罰球中至少要踢進(jìn) 3次”時(shí)), 即調(diào)節(jié)定向與任務(wù)定向的匹配可有效提高足球運(yùn)動(dòng)員的點(diǎn)球成功率(Plessner et al., 2009)。對(duì)籃球運(yùn)動(dòng)員“三分球”的研究也得到了相似的結(jié)果,處于調(diào)節(jié)匹配狀態(tài)的運(yùn)動(dòng)員的三分球成功率顯著高于非調(diào)節(jié)匹配狀態(tài)的運(yùn)動(dòng)員(Memmert et al., 2009)。
對(duì)于球類項(xiàng)目而言, 球能否命中目標(biāo)對(duì)運(yùn)動(dòng)員接下來(lái)的表現(xiàn)甚至整場(chǎng)比賽的進(jìn)程都可能產(chǎn)生至關(guān)重要的影響, 罰球成功與否對(duì)比賽勝負(fù)或運(yùn)動(dòng)員表現(xiàn)的預(yù)測(cè)率可達(dá)75%~85% (Kuss, Kluttig,& Stoll, 2007; Wood & Wilson, 2010)。因此, 利用調(diào)節(jié)匹配提高運(yùn)動(dòng)員進(jìn)球率(運(yùn)動(dòng)表現(xiàn))成為研究者關(guān)注的焦點(diǎn)之一。但目前研究中足球、籃球項(xiàng)目上出現(xiàn)的調(diào)節(jié)匹配效應(yīng)僅發(fā)生在較低水平的運(yùn)動(dòng)員群體中。對(duì)此, 有研究者認(rèn)為這并不表明調(diào)節(jié)匹配效應(yīng)受到運(yùn)動(dòng)水平的限制(如專家、新手在調(diào)節(jié)匹配效應(yīng)上存在不同等), 而更可能是由于運(yùn)動(dòng)項(xiàng)目的特點(diǎn)導(dǎo)致了對(duì)調(diào)節(jié)匹配效應(yīng)的低估或限制(Kutzner et al., 2013)。對(duì)于足球、籃球這類對(duì)抗性團(tuán)隊(duì)項(xiàng)目, 決定運(yùn)動(dòng)表現(xiàn)的因素有很多, 運(yùn)動(dòng)員會(huì)在各種因素的作用下對(duì)運(yùn)動(dòng)表現(xiàn)與比賽輸贏的關(guān)系發(fā)展出相應(yīng)的認(rèn)知建構(gòu)能力。高水平運(yùn)動(dòng)員可憑借這種認(rèn)知建構(gòu)能力(或積累的運(yùn)動(dòng)經(jīng)驗(yàn))對(duì)比賽目標(biāo)、輸贏關(guān)系等進(jìn)行重構(gòu), 對(duì)任務(wù)定向進(jìn)行合理認(rèn)知, 從而較少受到實(shí)驗(yàn)操作的影響(Dimotakis, Davison, & Hollenbeck, 2012; Folgado,Duarte, Marques, & Sampaio, 2015; Gharbi,Dardouri, Haj-Sassi, Chamari, & Souissi, 2015)。對(duì)此, 有研究者選擇了其他球類項(xiàng)目開展相關(guān)研究。例如一項(xiàng)針對(duì)高水平高爾夫(非對(duì)抗性球類項(xiàng)目)運(yùn)動(dòng)員的研究發(fā)現(xiàn), 當(dāng)操控任務(wù)定向使之與運(yùn)動(dòng)員調(diào)節(jié)定向相匹配時(shí), 運(yùn)動(dòng)員的擊球入洞成績(jī)可提高20% (Kutzner et al., 2013), 這提示在高水平運(yùn)動(dòng)員群體中亦存在調(diào)節(jié)匹配效應(yīng), 但存在運(yùn)動(dòng)項(xiàng)目間的差異。
關(guān)于調(diào)節(jié)匹配影響運(yùn)動(dòng)表現(xiàn)的心理機(jī)制, 相關(guān)研究發(fā)現(xiàn), 調(diào)節(jié)匹配狀態(tài)下的個(gè)體往往體驗(yàn)到一種“正確”的感受, 這種“正確感”會(huì)帶來(lái)積極的情緒體驗(yàn), 并且使個(gè)體更傾向于將這一情緒狀態(tài)(積極情緒)歸因于運(yùn)動(dòng)行為帶來(lái)的結(jié)果, 這種積極的心理狀態(tài)對(duì)行為表現(xiàn)可起到一定的促進(jìn)作用(Cheung, Gillebaart, Kroese, & De Ridder, 2014;Furley, Dicks, Stendtke, & Memmert, 2012; Jordet& Hartman, 2008; van Doorn, van Kleef, & van de Pligt, 2014; Wang, 2011)?;诖? 研究者們認(rèn)為調(diào)節(jié)匹配能夠促進(jìn)運(yùn)動(dòng)表現(xiàn)是由于調(diào)節(jié)定向與任務(wù)定向的匹配降低了運(yùn)動(dòng)員的焦慮水平, 促成良好的競(jìng)賽心理狀態(tài), 穩(wěn)定運(yùn)動(dòng)員的運(yùn)動(dòng)表現(xiàn), 促使其運(yùn)動(dòng)技能得以正常甚至超長(zhǎng)發(fā)揮。也就是說,調(diào)節(jié)匹配并不直接影響運(yùn)動(dòng)員的運(yùn)動(dòng)表現(xiàn), 而是為運(yùn)動(dòng)技能的發(fā)揮提供了一個(gè)良好的心理狀態(tài)前提或保障(Cheung et al., 2014; Dragos, 2014; Furley et al., 2012; Jordet & Hartman, 2008; Liu, Zhou,Liu, & Jack, 2012; Spiegel, Grant-Pillow, & Higgins,2004; van Doorn et al., 2014; Wang, 2011)。
近期研究還發(fā)現(xiàn), 調(diào)節(jié)匹配對(duì)運(yùn)動(dòng)表現(xiàn)的影響受到運(yùn)動(dòng)技能特征(如任務(wù)難度)的調(diào)節(jié)(Vogel& Genschow, 2013)。研究者以“調(diào)節(jié)匹配”和“任務(wù)難度” (設(shè)置點(diǎn)球距離:11 m為困難任務(wù), 7 m為簡(jiǎn)單任務(wù))為自變量, 點(diǎn)球成功率為因變量, 考察在不同任務(wù)難度下調(diào)節(jié)匹配對(duì)點(diǎn)球成功率(運(yùn)動(dòng)表現(xiàn))的影響。結(jié)果發(fā)現(xiàn), 調(diào)節(jié)匹配僅提高了運(yùn)動(dòng)員在簡(jiǎn)單任務(wù)中的點(diǎn)球成功率, 而對(duì)困難任務(wù)中的點(diǎn)球成功率沒有影響。其原因可能是, 難度越高,任務(wù)本身對(duì)運(yùn)動(dòng)員的技能需求也越高, 當(dāng)運(yùn)動(dòng)員的技能水平無(wú)法滿足任務(wù)難度的需求時(shí), 調(diào)節(jié)匹配對(duì)運(yùn)動(dòng)表現(xiàn)的積極影響受到技能水平的制約而難以形成, 但這一推斷還有待進(jìn)一步的檢驗(yàn)。
面對(duì)復(fù)雜多變的運(yùn)動(dòng)情境, 運(yùn)動(dòng)員決策策略的選擇可直接影響運(yùn)動(dòng)決策的效果, 因此, 如何在競(jìng)技情境中選擇最佳的決策策略, 產(chǎn)生最優(yōu)的決策效果, 也就成為運(yùn)動(dòng)心理研究的核心問題之一(遲立忠, 張力為, 2013)。調(diào)節(jié)匹配亦可對(duì)運(yùn)動(dòng)員決策策略的選擇產(chǎn)生影響, 而這種影響可能與比賽的進(jìn)程、決策任務(wù)的性質(zhì)(如定向方式)等有關(guān)。
有研究者考察了比賽不同時(shí)段運(yùn)動(dòng)員采用的決策策略差異。結(jié)果發(fā)現(xiàn), 在手球比賽中, 運(yùn)動(dòng)員決策策略的選擇會(huì)受到比賽進(jìn)程的影響。同其他時(shí)段相比, 運(yùn)動(dòng)員在下半場(chǎng)的第二階段更傾向于采用促進(jìn)定向策略, 如積極組織進(jìn)攻、拉開比分優(yōu)勢(shì)等, 并且獲得的決策效果更好。分析原因認(rèn)為, 該時(shí)段運(yùn)動(dòng)員對(duì)決策信息的加工更傾向于促進(jìn)定向(如此時(shí)不拼更待何時(shí)), 當(dāng)采用促進(jìn)定向策略時(shí), 可促成運(yùn)動(dòng)員調(diào)節(jié)定向與決策策略的匹配, 提高運(yùn)動(dòng)員對(duì)決策判斷的正確感, 改善決策效果(Debanne et al., 2014)。此外, 有研究發(fā)現(xiàn)面對(duì)不同性質(zhì)的運(yùn)動(dòng)任務(wù), 選擇與任務(wù)自身定向趨勢(shì)相一致的行為策略, 會(huì)獲得更好的行為效果。如籃球中的“三分投籃”任務(wù)更傾向于獲得性任務(wù)定向。當(dāng)運(yùn)動(dòng)員采用的決策策略(如促進(jìn)定向策略)與“三分投籃”任務(wù)定向方式相一致時(shí), 運(yùn)動(dòng)執(zhí)行中的調(diào)節(jié)匹配效應(yīng)得以增強(qiáng)(即促進(jìn)匹配下其行為表現(xiàn)更好); 而“足球點(diǎn)球”任務(wù)本身更傾向于規(guī)避性任務(wù)定向, 當(dāng)運(yùn)動(dòng)員采用的決策策略(如防御定向策略)與任務(wù)定向相一致時(shí), 調(diào)節(jié)匹配效應(yīng)則更突出(即防御匹配下其行為表現(xiàn)最佳) (Memmert et al., 2009; Plessner et al., 2009)。
雖然依據(jù)調(diào)節(jié)匹配理論, 個(gè)體選擇與自身定向方式相符的行為或決策策略時(shí), 決策效果更好(Ashraf & Thongpapanl, 2015; Memmert, Hüttermann,& Orliczek, 2013)。但在借助此理論考察調(diào)節(jié)匹配與復(fù)雜情境中決策行為(例如運(yùn)動(dòng)決策)的關(guān)系時(shí),還需考慮復(fù)雜的情境因素(Adiandari, 2014)。例如,研究者通過操控比賽視頻呈現(xiàn)時(shí)間對(duì)兩種運(yùn)動(dòng)決策情境(直覺決策、認(rèn)知決策)進(jìn)行研究。結(jié)果發(fā)現(xiàn),被試在比賽情境畫面較長(zhǎng)(如1000 ms)、獲取線索較為充分的認(rèn)知決策情境下的決策效果好于比賽情境畫面較短(如 400 ms)、獲取線索較不充分的直覺決策情境(王斌, 2004)。這是因?yàn)檫\(yùn)動(dòng)員在進(jìn)行直覺決策時(shí), 由于時(shí)間的緊迫性和信息的不充分性, 個(gè)體面對(duì)的決策困難較大, 往往基于直覺思維進(jìn)行判斷, 在此種情況下, 直覺性思維造成的非理性偏差往往會(huì)導(dǎo)致較差的決策效果(王洪彪, 2013; Epley & Gilovich, 2006)。劉茜(2014)在此基礎(chǔ)上考慮了調(diào)節(jié)匹配的作用, 以調(diào)節(jié)匹配范式和運(yùn)動(dòng)決策時(shí)間掩蔽范式探討調(diào)節(jié)匹配的影響。結(jié)果表明, 調(diào)節(jié)匹配促進(jìn)了運(yùn)動(dòng)員對(duì)直覺決策信息的加工, 提高了直覺決策的效果。這可能是由于處于匹配狀態(tài)下的運(yùn)動(dòng)員注意范圍更廣,可在關(guān)注決策任務(wù)的同時(shí)分配更多的注意資源關(guān)注場(chǎng)上情境(即決策情境)的變化, 進(jìn)而對(duì)決策信息進(jìn)行更為全面的建構(gòu)與評(píng)價(jià), 修正由直覺性決策思維造成的非理性偏差, 產(chǎn)生更多更流暢的問題解決策略并進(jìn)行更為合理的決策, 獲得更好的決策效果(Bullens, van Harreveld, F?rster, & Higgins,2014; Grimm, Markman, Maddox, & Baldwin,2008)。
鍛煉既可以預(yù)防或延緩疾病的發(fā)生與發(fā)展,也可以有效促進(jìn)心理健康(Daenen, Varkey, Kellmann,& Nijs, 2015; Graham, Sirard, & Neumark, 2011;Umeda, Lee, Marino, & Hilliard, 2016)。隨著鍛煉心理與行為研究的深入, 有關(guān)調(diào)節(jié)匹配對(duì)鍛煉行為影響的研究也逐步展開, 其中調(diào)節(jié)匹配與鍛煉動(dòng)機(jī)、鍛煉行為的關(guān)系受到較多關(guān)注。
不同個(gè)體的鍛煉動(dòng)機(jī)不同, 有的鍛煉者是因?yàn)樵阱憻掃^程中體驗(yàn)到了“滿足感”或“享受感”,他們更關(guān)注內(nèi)在的結(jié)果體驗(yàn)(以內(nèi)在動(dòng)機(jī)為特征,如能力的提高); 有的鍛煉者則更關(guān)注鍛煉對(duì)保持“健康”或“維持自身的好身材”的積極影響, 即更關(guān)注外在的結(jié)果體驗(yàn)(以外在動(dòng)機(jī)為特征, 如通過鍛煉獲得某種益處) (Buckworth, Lee, Regan, Schneider,& DiClemente, 2007; Vansteenkiste, Matos, Lens, &Soenens, 2007)。有研究者向206名18~69歲的久坐少動(dòng)的研究參與者(對(duì)其進(jìn)行定向方式的測(cè)量)隨機(jī)提供促進(jìn)性鍛煉信息(如鍛煉可以提高鍛煉者的力量和耐力)或防御性鍛煉信息(如缺乏鍛煉會(huì)使鍛煉者增加體重), 以結(jié)果體驗(yàn)感受(內(nèi)在、外在)為測(cè)量指標(biāo)。結(jié)果發(fā)現(xiàn), 相比于不匹配狀態(tài)(向促進(jìn)定向的鍛煉者提供防御性鍛煉信息或向防御定向鍛煉者提供促進(jìn)性鍛煉信息), 調(diào)節(jié)匹配促進(jìn)了鍛煉者對(duì)鍛煉行為的結(jié)果體驗(yàn)感受, 即向防御定向鍛煉者提供防御性鍛煉信息, 增強(qiáng)了其外在的結(jié)果體驗(yàn)感受; 向促進(jìn)定向鍛煉者提供促進(jìn)性鍛煉信息, 則提高了其內(nèi)在結(jié)果體驗(yàn)感受(Latimer et al., 2008)。這提示, 不同定向的鍛煉者面對(duì)不同調(diào)節(jié)定向的鍛煉信息時(shí), 即使這些信息傳達(dá)的內(nèi)容是積極正面的, 包含了促進(jìn)鍛煉者實(shí)施鍛煉行為的因素, 但因鍛煉者自身定向的不同, 其結(jié)果體驗(yàn)仍會(huì)存在差異。
進(jìn)一步的研究發(fā)現(xiàn), 與不匹配的鍛煉者相比,處于調(diào)節(jié)匹配狀態(tài)下的鍛煉者能夠在鍛煉結(jié)束后更多地回顧在鍛煉過程中體驗(yàn)到的積極感受, 有效地提高了鍛煉者參與鍛煉的動(dòng)機(jī), 調(diào)節(jié)匹配狀態(tài)下的鍛煉者也確實(shí)表示更愿意堅(jiān)持鍛煉行為(Daryanto et al., 2010; Gallagher & Updegraff, 2011;Latimer et al., 2008; Pfeffer, 2013)。也就是說, 鍛煉信息傳達(dá)的方式符合鍛煉者的定向方式, 會(huì)提高鍛煉者對(duì)鍛煉行為的評(píng)價(jià), 改善鍛煉者對(duì)鍛煉行為的態(tài)度, 進(jìn)而促使其鍛煉行為得以發(fā)生和維持(Higgins, Cesario, Hagiwara, Spiegel, & Pittman,2010; Hui, Molden, & Finkel, 2013)。
調(diào)節(jié)匹配理論的提出打破了鍛煉動(dòng)機(jī)的單一模型, 對(duì)“提供積極信息反饋時(shí)其鍛煉動(dòng)機(jī)最佳”這一觀點(diǎn)提出了質(zhì)疑(Sebire, Standage, & Vansteenkiste,2011; Sebire, Standage, Gillison, & Vansteenkiste,2013), 更加強(qiáng)調(diào)鍛煉信息與鍛煉者調(diào)節(jié)定向的關(guān)系。調(diào)節(jié)匹配理論認(rèn)為, 即使向鍛煉者提供積極的鍛煉信息, 但能否引發(fā)鍛煉者的最佳動(dòng)機(jī)水平還取決于鍛煉信息與鍛煉者調(diào)節(jié)定向是否匹配。相關(guān)研究已發(fā)現(xiàn), 相比于不匹配狀態(tài), 調(diào)節(jié)匹配下的鍛煉者的鍛煉動(dòng)機(jī)更強(qiáng)。對(duì)于促進(jìn)定向的鍛煉者, 提供促進(jìn)性的積極信息反饋(如通過鍛煉可以獲得能力的提升)會(huì)增強(qiáng)其“獲得成功”的動(dòng)機(jī);而對(duì)于防御定向的鍛煉者, 提供防御性的積極信息反饋(如通過鍛煉可以避免生病)會(huì)增強(qiáng)其“規(guī)避風(fēng)險(xiǎn)”的動(dòng)機(jī)(Jarzebowski, Palermo, & van de Berg,2012; Otto, Markman, Gureckis, & Love, 2010), 即調(diào)節(jié)匹配時(shí)(促進(jìn)匹配、防御匹配), 鍛煉動(dòng)機(jī)均會(huì)得到提高, 只是鍛煉動(dòng)機(jī)的方向以及鍛煉者對(duì)鍛煉行為的感受存在差異, 但對(duì)鍛煉行為的發(fā)生和維持均具有促進(jìn)作用。
此外, 還有研究發(fā)現(xiàn)當(dāng)提供給鍛煉者的鍛煉信息與鍛煉者自身的調(diào)節(jié)定向匹配時(shí), 鍛煉者對(duì)鍛煉信息的采納程度更高, 更愿意接受鍛煉的建議, 并以此作為指導(dǎo)自己鍛煉行為的依據(jù), 進(jìn)而達(dá)成更為積極的反饋效果, 提高鍛煉動(dòng)機(jī), 維持鍛煉行為(Mannetti, Brizi, Giacomantonio, & Higgins,2013)。因此, 鍛煉者在進(jìn)行鍛煉時(shí), 可以充分利用調(diào)節(jié)匹配的優(yōu)勢(shì), 先了解自身的調(diào)節(jié)定向, 后采用與其相符的鍛煉信息、行為策略, 進(jìn)而增強(qiáng)鍛煉動(dòng)機(jī)、提高鍛煉熱情、堅(jiān)持鍛煉行為。
目前競(jìng)技運(yùn)動(dòng)領(lǐng)域關(guān)于調(diào)節(jié)匹配的研究主要圍繞調(diào)節(jié)匹配與運(yùn)動(dòng)表現(xiàn)和運(yùn)動(dòng)決策的關(guān)系展開,發(fā)現(xiàn)調(diào)節(jié)匹配可以提高運(yùn)動(dòng)員的運(yùn)動(dòng)表現(xiàn), 增強(qiáng)運(yùn)動(dòng)員的決策效果(Kutzner et al., 2013; Vogel &Genschow, 2013)。鍛煉領(lǐng)域的調(diào)節(jié)匹配研究主要圍繞調(diào)節(jié)匹配與鍛煉動(dòng)機(jī)及鍛煉行為的關(guān)系展開,發(fā)現(xiàn)調(diào)節(jié)匹配可提高鍛煉動(dòng)機(jī), 改善鍛煉行為(Presseau et al., 2014; Sheeran, Harris, & Epton,2014)。故此, 實(shí)踐中可根據(jù)運(yùn)動(dòng)個(gè)體的調(diào)節(jié)定向選擇相應(yīng)的策略, 促成調(diào)節(jié)匹配, 縮短當(dāng)前心理與行為狀態(tài)或目標(biāo)狀態(tài)的距離, 節(jié)省認(rèn)知資源,進(jìn)而更加有效地完成運(yùn)動(dòng)行為或鍛煉行為。
基于運(yùn)動(dòng)領(lǐng)域調(diào)節(jié)匹配的研究現(xiàn)況, 未來(lái)可側(cè)重從以下三方面對(duì)已有研究進(jìn)行擴(kuò)展和深化。
第一, 充實(shí)運(yùn)動(dòng)情境下調(diào)節(jié)匹配的研究?jī)?nèi)容。運(yùn)動(dòng)行為受到諸多因素的影響, 其中既有外部因素(如項(xiàng)目特征、比賽對(duì)手、雙方比分、比賽進(jìn)程等), 也有內(nèi)部因素(如人格、認(rèn)知、反應(yīng)傾向、情緒、智力等) (Hagtvet & Hanin, 2007; Kempton,Kennedy, & Coutts, 2016; McCarthy, 2011; Plessner& Haar, 2006; Szabo & Müller, 2016), 考慮到這些內(nèi)外因素的作用, 調(diào)節(jié)匹配與運(yùn)動(dòng)表現(xiàn)、決策質(zhì)量的關(guān)系變得復(fù)雜多樣?,F(xiàn)有研究已經(jīng)發(fā)現(xiàn), 調(diào)節(jié)匹配可以提高運(yùn)動(dòng)員的運(yùn)動(dòng)表現(xiàn), 增強(qiáng)運(yùn)動(dòng)員的決策效果, 但受到運(yùn)動(dòng)項(xiàng)目和運(yùn)動(dòng)難度的調(diào)節(jié)(Kutzner et al., 2013; Vogel & Genschow, 2013)。調(diào)節(jié)匹配可提高鍛煉動(dòng)機(jī), 改善鍛煉行為, 但會(huì)受到社會(huì)支持的影響, 如當(dāng)重要他人對(duì)鍛煉者的鍛煉行為持反對(duì)意見時(shí), 即使鍛煉者處于調(diào)節(jié)匹配狀態(tài), 仍難以促成鍛煉行為的發(fā)生或維持(Presseau et al., 2014; Sheeran et al., 2014)??梢?為準(zhǔn)確揭示運(yùn)動(dòng)情境下調(diào)節(jié)匹配與行為表現(xiàn)的關(guān)系, 需要將一些重要的運(yùn)動(dòng)情境因素和運(yùn)動(dòng)員個(gè)人或群體特征納入其中, 并對(duì)多種因素間的相互關(guān)系及協(xié)同作用進(jìn)行深入研究, 進(jìn)而更加客觀、全面地評(píng)價(jià)運(yùn)動(dòng)領(lǐng)域中的調(diào)節(jié)匹配效應(yīng), 避免夸大調(diào)節(jié)匹配對(duì)運(yùn)動(dòng)行為的影響。
第二, 探究運(yùn)動(dòng)情境下調(diào)節(jié)匹配的作用機(jī)制。目前對(duì)調(diào)節(jié)匹配效應(yīng)的解釋認(rèn)為, 不同調(diào)節(jié)定向的運(yùn)動(dòng)個(gè)體在面對(duì)運(yùn)動(dòng)任務(wù)目標(biāo)時(shí)會(huì)產(chǎn)生不同的行為狀態(tài)(如匹配或不匹配), 由此影響個(gè)體對(duì)運(yùn)動(dòng)競(jìng)賽行為或鍛煉行為的看法, 導(dǎo)致其行為表現(xiàn)出現(xiàn)差異。例如, 不匹配的行為狀態(tài)會(huì)使個(gè)體產(chǎn)生負(fù)性的情緒感受, 對(duì)運(yùn)動(dòng)行為產(chǎn)生消極看法, 進(jìn)而無(wú)法借助自主行為活動(dòng)做出迅速、反射性的行為反應(yīng), 對(duì)個(gè)體的行為表現(xiàn)起到一定的阻礙作用(Furley et al., 2012; Jordet & Hartman, 2008;Wang, 2011)。而調(diào)節(jié)匹配狀態(tài)下的運(yùn)動(dòng)個(gè)體往往體驗(yàn)到一種“正確”的感受, 這種“正確感”會(huì)帶來(lái)積極的情緒體驗(yàn), 對(duì)行為表現(xiàn)可起到一定的促進(jìn)作用(Cheung et al., 2014; Unkelbach, Plessner, &Memmert, 2009; van Doorn et al., 2014)。面對(duì)復(fù)雜多變的運(yùn)動(dòng)情境和運(yùn)動(dòng)行為, 這一解釋似過于簡(jiǎn)單。研究者提出, 調(diào)節(jié)匹配在提高正性情緒體驗(yàn)的基礎(chǔ)上、亦可以通過增強(qiáng)動(dòng)機(jī)水平或擴(kuò)寬注意范圍等來(lái)促進(jìn)個(gè)體行為的改善(F?rster & Dannenberg,2010; Idson, Liberman, & Higgins, 2004; McAuley,Henry, Wedd, Pleskac, & Cesario, 2012; Memmert,Unkelbach, & Ganns, 2010; Unkelbach et al.,2009 )。故此, 后續(xù)研究應(yīng)在理解運(yùn)動(dòng)行為理論的基礎(chǔ)上, 充分考慮運(yùn)動(dòng)情境的復(fù)雜性和多樣性,在現(xiàn)有行為研究結(jié)論的基礎(chǔ)上, 可引入認(rèn)知神經(jīng)的相關(guān)指標(biāo), 逐步實(shí)現(xiàn)從行為及神經(jīng)生物兩個(gè)層次對(duì)運(yùn)動(dòng)情境下調(diào)節(jié)匹配作用機(jī)制的闡釋, 這樣既能避免對(duì)運(yùn)動(dòng)情境下調(diào)節(jié)匹配作用機(jī)制的簡(jiǎn)單化解釋, 深入了解運(yùn)動(dòng)領(lǐng)域中調(diào)節(jié)匹配的內(nèi)在機(jī)制, 同時(shí)避免可能出現(xiàn)的理論偏差, 亦可促進(jìn)對(duì)調(diào)節(jié)匹配一般作用機(jī)制的探討。
第三, 完善運(yùn)動(dòng)情境下調(diào)節(jié)匹配的研究方法。競(jìng)技運(yùn)動(dòng)中調(diào)節(jié)匹配的研究主要通過操縱調(diào)節(jié)定向與任務(wù)定向之間的關(guān)系來(lái)進(jìn)行。目前運(yùn)動(dòng)員的調(diào)節(jié)定向主要采用量表進(jìn)行測(cè)量, 通過對(duì)促進(jìn)定向與防御定向兩個(gè)分量表的差值進(jìn)行中位數(shù)分割(中位數(shù)以上為促進(jìn)定向、中位數(shù)以下為防御定向), 或通過采用差值大者為促進(jìn)定向、差值小者為防御定向的模糊劃分方式來(lái)評(píng)定運(yùn)動(dòng)員的調(diào)節(jié)定向(Memmert et al., 2009; Plessner et al.,2009)。這些評(píng)定標(biāo)準(zhǔn)較為模糊且易導(dǎo)致選擇性偏差, 后續(xù)研究可通過采用標(biāo)準(zhǔn) Z分?jǐn)?shù)對(duì)測(cè)量數(shù)值進(jìn)行矯正, 使調(diào)節(jié)定向的評(píng)價(jià)標(biāo)準(zhǔn)更加客觀、明確。另外, 在操縱任務(wù)定向時(shí), 大部分研究者會(huì)采用“指導(dǎo)語(yǔ)誘發(fā)”的方式。但這種操縱方式通常啟動(dòng)強(qiáng)度不足、生態(tài)學(xué)效度不高, 目前一些調(diào)節(jié)匹配研究未取得預(yù)期結(jié)果或與采用“指導(dǎo)語(yǔ)誘發(fā)”方式有關(guān), 后續(xù)研究可嘗試采用“多重任務(wù)啟動(dòng)”方式, 即采用情緒誘發(fā)與指導(dǎo)語(yǔ)誘發(fā)等多種形式共同啟動(dòng)任務(wù)定向(劉茜, 2014)。此外, 目前對(duì)鍛煉信息的誘導(dǎo)方式尚缺少客觀性指標(biāo)的操控檢驗(yàn),致使可能存在一定的實(shí)驗(yàn)偏差。后續(xù)研究或可通過大量訪談構(gòu)建出相關(guān)研究均可采用的、且具有良好效度的實(shí)驗(yàn)材料(促進(jìn)性鍛煉信息、防御性鍛煉信息)。
遲立忠, 張力為. (2013). 當(dāng)代運(yùn)動(dòng)心理學(xué)進(jìn)展: 競(jìng)技心理.北京體育大學(xué)學(xué)報(bào), 36(9), 49–55, 63.
段錦云, 周冉, 陸文娟, 李晶, 朱宜超. (2013). 不同反應(yīng)線索條件下調(diào)節(jié)匹配對(duì)建議采納的影響.心理學(xué)報(bào),45(1),104–113.
劉茜. (2014).調(diào)節(jié)匹配與壓力及整體-局部加工方式對(duì)籃球運(yùn)動(dòng)員決策的影響(碩士學(xué)位論文). 北京體育大學(xué).
王斌. (2004). 男子手球運(yùn)動(dòng)員模擬運(yùn)動(dòng)情境中認(rèn)知決策與直覺決策的差異.體育科學(xué), 24(6), 45–48.
王洪彪. (2013). 運(yùn)動(dòng)決策情境中認(rèn)知加工理論模型的初步構(gòu)建.沈陽(yáng)體育學(xué)院學(xué)報(bào),32(3), 28–32.
王懷勇. (2011).決策過程中的調(diào)節(jié)匹配效應(yīng)研究(博士學(xué)位論文). 華東師范大學(xué), 上海.
Adiandari, A. M. (2014). Intuitive decision making: The intuition concept in decision making process.International Journal of Business and Behavioral Sciences, 4(7), 1–12.
Ashraf, A. R., & Thongpapanl, N. (2015). Connecting with and converting shoppers into customers: Investigating the role of regulatory fit in the online customer's decisionmaking process.Journal of Interactive Marketing, 32,13–25.
Barber, S. J., & Mather, M. (2013).Stereotype threat can both enhance and impair older adults’ memory.Psychological Science, 24(12), 2522–2529.
Buckworth, J., Lee, R. E., Regan, G., Schneider, L. K., &DiClemente, C. C. (2007). Decomposing intrinsic and extrinsic motivation for exercise: Application to stages of motivational readiness.Psychology of Sport and Exercise,8(4), 441–461.
Bullens, L., van Harreveld, F., F?rster, J., & Higgins, T. E.(2014). How decision reversibility affects motivation.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143(2),835–849.
Chatterjee, S., Roy, R., & Malshe, A. V. (2011). The role of regulatory fit on the attraction effect.Journal of Consumer Psychology, 21(4), 473–481.
Chen, L. (2014). A study on the effects of regulatory fit and framing.Applied Mechanics and Materials, 644–650,6289–6292.
Cheung, T. T., Gillebaart, M., Kroese, F., & De Ridder, D.(2014). Why are people with high self-control happier?The effect of trait self-control on happiness as mediated by regulatory focus.Front in Psychology, 5, 722.
Daenen, L., Varkey, E., Kellmann, M., & Nijs, J. (2015).Exercise, not to exercise, or how to exercise in patients with chronic pain? Applying science to practice.The Clinical Journal of Pain, 31(2), 108–114.
Daryanto, A., de Ruyter, K., Wetzels, M., & Patterson, P. G.(2010). Service firms and customer loyalty programs: A regulatory fit perspective of reward preferences in a health club setting.Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science,38(5), 604–616.
Debanne, T., Angel, V., & Fontayne, P. (2014). Decisionmaking during games by professional handball coaches using regulatory focus theory.Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 26(1), 111–124.
Dimotakis, N., Davison, R. B., & Hollenbeck, J. R. (2012).Team structure and regulatory focus: The impact of regulatory fit on team dynamic.The Journal of Applied Psychology, 97(2), 421–434.
Dragos, P. F. (2014). Study regarding the role of motivation in the sport performance activities.Baltic Journal of Health & Physical Activity, 6(1), 48–55.
Epley, N., & Gilovich, T. (2006). The anchoring-and-adjustment heuristic.Psychological Science, 17(4), 311–318.
Folgado, H., Duarte, R., Marques, P., & Sampaio, J. (2015).The effects of congested fixtures period on tactical and physical performance in elite football.Journal of Sports Sciences, 33(12), 1238–1247.
F?rster, J., & Dannenberg, L. (2010). Glomo says: A systems account of global versus local processing.Psychological Inquiry, 21(3), 175–197.
Fuglestad, P. T., Rothman, A. J., & Jeffery, R. W. (2013). The effects of regulatory focus on responding to and avoiding slips in a longitudinal study of smoking cessation.Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 35(5), 426–435.
Furley, P., Dicks, M., Stendtke, F., & Memmert, D. (2012).“Get it out the way, the wait's killing me.” hastening and hiding during soccer penalty kick.Psychology of Sport &Exercise, 13(4), 454–465.
Gallagher, K. M., & Updegraff, J. A. (2011). When “fit”leads to fit, and when “fit” leads to fat: How message framing and intrinsic vs extrinsic exercise outcomes interact in promoting physical activity.Psychology and Health, 26(7), 819–834.
Gharbi, D., Dardouri, W., Haj-Sassi, R., Chamari, K., &Souissi, N. (2015). Aerobic and anaerobic determinants of repeated sprint ability in team sports athletes.Biology of Sport, 32(3), 207–212.
Graham, D. J., Sirard, J. R., & Neumark, S. D. (2011).Adolescents’ attitudes toward sports, exercise, and fitness predict physical activity 5 and 10 years later.Preventive Medicine, 52(2), 130–132.
Grimm, L. R., Markman, A. B., Maddox, W. T., & Baldwin,G. C. (2008). Differential effects of regulatory fit on category learning.Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,44(3), 920–927.
Hagtvet, K. A., & Hanin, Y. L. (2007). Consistency of performance-related emotions in elite athletes: Generalizability theory applied to the IZOF model.Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 8(1), 47–72.
Higgins, E. T. (2000). Making a good decision: Value from fit.American Psychologist, 55(11), 1217–1230.
Higgins, E. T., Cesario, J., Hagiwara, N., Spiegel, S., &Pittman, T. (2010). Increasing or decreasing interest in activities: The role of regulatory fit.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99(4), 559–572.
Higgins, E. T., Idson, L. C., Freitas, A. L., Spiegel, S., &Molden, D. C. (2003). Transfer of value from fit.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(6), 1140–1153.
Hui, C. M., Molden, D. C., & Finkel, E. J. (2013). Loving Freedom: Concerns with promotion or prevention and the role of autonomy in relationship well-being.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 105(1), 61–85.
Idson, L. C., Liberman, N., & Higgins, E. T. (2004).Imagining how you'd feel: The role of motivational experiences from regulatory fit.Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30(7), 926–937.
Jarzebowski, A. M., Palermo, J., & van de Berg, R. (2012).When feedback is not enough: The impact of regulatory fit on motivation after positive feedback.International Coaching Psychology Review, 7(1), 14–32.
Jordet, G., & Hartmen, E. (2008). Avoidance motivation and choking under pressure in soccer penalty shootouts.Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 30(4), 450–457.
Kao, D. T. (2012). Exploring the effect of regulatory focus on ad attitudes: The moderating roles of message sidedness and argument quality.International Journal of Psychology,47(2), 142–153.
Keller, J., & Bless, H. (2006). Regulatory fit and cognitive performance: The interactive effect of chronic and situationally induced self-regulatory mechanisms on test performance.European Journal of Social Psychology,36(3), 393–405.
Kempton, T., Kennedy, N., & Coutts, A. J. (2016). The expected value of possession in professional rugby league match-play.Journal of Sports Sciences, 34(7), 645–650.
Kuss, O., Kluttig, A., & Stoll, O. (2007). ‘‘The fouled player shouldn’t take the penalty himself’’: An empirical investigation of an old German football myth.Journal of Sports Sciences, 25(9), 963–967.
Kutzner, F. L. W., F?rderer, S., & Plessner, H. (2013).Regulatory fit improves putting in top golfers.Sport,Exercise, and Performance Psychology, 2(2), 130–137.
Latimer, A. E., Rivers, S. E., Rench, T. A., Katulak, N. A.,Hicks, A., … Salovey, P. (2008). A field experiment testing the utility of regulatory fit messages for promoting physical activity.Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,44(3), 826–832.
Liu, W., Zhou, C. L., Ji, L., & Watson, J. C. (2012). The effect of goal setting difficulty on serving success in table tennis and the mediating mechanism of self-regulation.Journal of Human Kinetics, 33, 173–185.
Ludolph, R., & Schulz, P. J. (2015). Does regulatory fit lead to more effective health communication? A systematic review.Social Science and Medicine, 128, 142–150.
Mannetti, L., Brizi, A., Giacomantonio, M., & Higgins, E. T.(2013). Framing political messages to fit the audience's regulatory orientation: How to improve the efficacy of the same message content.PLoS One, 8(10), e77040.
McAuley, J. D., Henry, M. J., Wedd, A., Pleskac, T. J., &Cesario, J. (2012). Effects of musicality and motivational orientation on auditory category learning: A test of a regulatory-fit hypothesis.Memory and Cognition, 40(2),231–251.
McCarthy, P. J. (2011). Positive emotion in sport performance:Current status and future directions.International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 4(1), 50–69.
Memmert, D., Hüttermann, S., & Orliczek, J. (2013). Decide like Lionel Messi! The impact of regulatory focus on divergent thinking in sports.Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 43(10), 2163–2167.
Memmert, D., Plessner, H., Hüttermann, S., Froese, G.,Peterh?nsel, C., & Unkelbach, C. (2015).Collective fit increases team performances: extending regulatory fit from individuals to dyadic teams.Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 45(5), 274–281.
Memmert, D., Unkelbach, C., & Ganns, S. (2010). The impact of regulatory fit on performance in an inattentional blindness paradigm.The Journal of General Psychology,137(2), 129–139.
Otto, A. R., Markman, A. B., Gureckis, T. M., & Love, B. C.(2010). Regulatory fit and systematic exploration in a dynamic decision-making environment.Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36(3),797–804.
Park, G., van Dyne, L., & Ilgen, D. (2013). Satisfaction pursuing approach and avoidance goals: Effects of regulatory fit and individual temperaments.Motivation &Emotion, 37(3), 424–432.
Pfeffer, I. (2013). Regulatory fit messages and physical activity motivation.Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 35(2), 119–131.
Plessner, H., & Haar, T. (2006). Sports performance judgments from a social cognitive perspective.Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 7(6), 555–575.
Plessner, H., Unkelbach, C., Memmert, D., Baltes, A., &Kolb, A. (2009). Regulatory fit as a determinant of sport performance: How to succeed in a soccer penalty-shooting.Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 10, 108–115.
Presseau, J., Johnston, M., Francis, J. J., Hrisos, S., Stamp,E., Steen, N., … Eccles, M. P. (2014). Theory-based predictors of multiple clinician behaviors in the management of diabetes.Journal of Behavioral Medicine,37(4), 607–620.
Righetti, F., Francesca, C., & Rusbult, C. (2011). The benefits of interpersonal regulatory fit for individual goal pursuit.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,101(4), 720–736.
Scholer, A. A., Ozaki, Y., & Higgins, E. T. (2014). Inflating and deflating the self: Sustaining motivational concerns through self-evaluation.Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 51, 60–73.
Scott, G. G., Sereno, S. C., & O'Donnell, P. J. (2012). Is a mean machine better than a dependable drive? It’s geared toward your regulatory focus.Frontiers in Psychology, 3,268.
Sebire, S. J., Standage, M., & Vansteenkiste, M. (2011).Predicting objectively assessed physical activity from the content and regulation of exercise goals: Evidence for a mediational model.Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 33(2), 175–197.
Sebire, S. J., Standage, M., Gillison, F. B., & Vansteenkiste,M. (2013). "Coveting thy neighbour's legs": A qualitative study of exercisers' experiences of intrinsic and extrinsic goal pursuit.Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology,35(3), 308–321.
Sheeran, P., Harris, P. R., & Epton, T. (2014). Does heightening risk appraisals change people's intentions and behavior? A meta-analysis of experimental studies.Psychological Bulletin, 140(2), 511–543.
Spiegel, S., Grant-Pillow, H., & Higgins, E. T. (2004). How regulatory fit enhances motivational strength during goal pursuit.European Journal of Social Psychology, 34(1),39–54.
Szabo, A., & Müller, A. (2016). Coaches' attitudes towards placebo interventions in sport.European Journal of Sport Science, 16(3), 293–300.
Umeda, M., Lee, W., Marino, C. A., & Hilliard, S. C. (2016).Influence of moderate intensity physical activity levels and gender on conditioned pain modulation.Journal of Sports Sciences, 34(5), 467–476.
Unkelbach, C., Plessner, H., & Memmert, D. (2009). “Fit” in sports: Self-regulation and athletic performances. In J. P.Forgas, R. F. Baumeister, & D. M. Tice (Eds.),Psychology of self-regulation: Cognitive, affective, and motivational processes. (pp. 93–105). New York: Psychology Press.
van Doorn, E. A., van Kleef, G. A., & van der Pligt, J. (2014).How instructors’ emotional expressions shape students’learning performance: The roles of anger, happiness, and regulatory focus.Journal of Experimental Psychology:General, 143(3), 980–984.
Vansteenkiste, M., Matos, M., Lens, W., & Soenens, B.(2007). Understanding the impact of intrinsic versus extrinsic goal framing on exercise performance: The conflicting role of task and ego involvement.Psychology of Sport & Exercise, 8(5), 771–794.
Vaughn, L. A. (2010).Regulatory fit and persuasion through advocacy messages and narratives. New York: Nova Science Publishers.
Vaughn, L. A., Harkness, A. R., & Clark, E. K. (2010). The effect of incidental experiences of regulatory fit on trust.Personal Relationships, 17(1), 57–69.
Vogel, T., & Genschow, O. (2013). When do chronic differences in self-regulation count? Regulatory focus effects in easy and difficult soccer tasks.Journal of Sport& Exercise Psychology, 35(2), 216–220.
Wang, X. (2011). The role of anticipated negative emotions and past behavior in individuals' physical activity intentions and behaviors.Psychology of Sport & Exercise,12(3), 300–305.
Wood, G., & Wilson, M. R. (2010). A moving goalkeeper distracts penalty takers and impairs shooting accuracy.Journal of Sports Sciences, 28(9), 937–946.
Worthy, D. A., Maddox, W. T., & Markman, A. B. (2007).Regulatory fit effects in a choice task.Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14(6), 1125–1132.