• <tr id="yyy80"></tr>
  • <sup id="yyy80"></sup>
  • <tfoot id="yyy80"><noscript id="yyy80"></noscript></tfoot>
  • 99热精品在线国产_美女午夜性视频免费_国产精品国产高清国产av_av欧美777_自拍偷自拍亚洲精品老妇_亚洲熟女精品中文字幕_www日本黄色视频网_国产精品野战在线观看 ?

    Partial Masquerading and Background Matching in Two Asian Box Turtle Species (Cuora spp.)

    2022-09-27 03:10:52RongpingBUFanrongXIAOGeorgeLOVELLJichaoWANGandHaitaoSHI
    Asian Herpetological Research 2022年3期

    Rongping BU ,Fanrong XIAO* ,P.George LOVELL ,Jichao WANG and Haitao SHI*

    1 Ministry of Education Key Laboratory for Ecology of Tropical Islands, Key Laboratory of Tropical Animal and Plant Ecology of Hainan Province,College of Life Sciences,Hainan Normal University,Haikou 571158,Hainan,China

    2 Division of Psychology and Forensic Sciences,School of Applied Sciences,Abertay University,Dundee DD1 1HG,UK

    Abstract Animals living in heterogeneous natural environments adopt different camouflage strategies against different backgrounds,and behavioral adaptation is crucial for their survival.However,studies of camouflage strategies have not always quantified the effect of multiple strategies used together.In the present study,we used a human visual model to quantify similarities in color and shape between the carapace patterns of two Cuora species and their preferred habitats.Our results showed that the color of the middle stripe on the carapace of Cuora galbinifrons(Indochinese box turtle) was significantly similar to the color of their preferred substrates.Meanwhile,the middle stripe on the carapace of C.mouhotii (keeled box turtle) contrasted more with their preferred substrates,and the side stripe matched most closely with the environment.Furthermore,the carapace side stripe of C.galbinifrons and the carapace middle stripe of C.mouhotii highly contrasted with their preferred substrates.We quantified the similarity in shape between the highcontrast stripes of both Cuora species and leaves from their habitats.The carapace middle stripe of C.mouhotii was most similar in shape to leaves from the broadleaves substrate,and the carapace side stripe of C.galbinifrons was the most similar in shape to leaves from the bamboo-leaves substrate.We determined that these species adopt partial masquerading when their entire carapace is exposed and partially match their background when they semi-cover themselves in leaf litter.To the best of our knowledge,this is the first study to demonstrate that partial masquerading and background matching improve the camouflage effect of Asian box turtles in their preferred habitats.This is a novel study focusing on the influence of the shape and color of individual carapace segments on reducing detectability and recognition.

    Keywords camouflage,Cuora galbinifrons,Cuora mouhotii,patterns,protective coloration

    1.Introduction

    Camouflage,the phenomenon in which the characteristics of an object or organism are arranged to prevent detection or recognition by an observer,is a common anti-predator strategy employed by many animals (Endler,1978;Merilaita and Lind,2005;Stevens and Merilaita,2011) and is crucial for survival in many species (Cott,1940;Ruxtonet al.,2004).Background matching,an important camouflaging strategy that helps an animal reduce detection by predators (Stevenset al.,2015;Stevens and Merilaita,2011),refers to the matching of the animals’ color,lightness,orientation,and pattern with their background (Kanget al.,2012;Marshallet al.,2016;Trosciankoet al.,2016).Masquerading animals appear to closely resemble inedible or inanimate objects,such as twigs,leaves,stones,or bird droppings (Endler,1981;Skelhornet al.,2010a),preventing predators from detecting or recognizing them.

    However,it is difficult to determine whether a predator has detected and misidentified a camouflaged individual (due to masquerade) or entirely failed to detect the prey individual (due to background matching and disruptive coloration) (Skelhornet al.,2010a;Skelhorn and Ruxton,2011).Thus,considering these two camouflage strategies separately is neither a requirement nor a realistic interpretation of the real world (Stevenset al.,2011).Prey animals can exploit background matching and masquerading simultaneously to increase the anti-predation benefits of the strategies.Under these circumstances,the benefit and impact of each strategy may differ,as would the overall effect of the combined strategies (Hallet al.,2017).

    To maximize the effectiveness of the camouflage,animals likely choose microhabitats that closely complement their color and shape (Lovellet al.,2013;Priceet al.,2019;Stevens and Ruxton,2018).For example,the common potoo bird (Nyctibius griseus) selects perching sites that enhance its background matching and masquerading effects (Cestariet al.,2018).Furthermore,many animals decorate themselves or modify their habitats to better conceal themselves (Gómezet al.,2018;Marshallet al.,2016;Suzuki and Sakurai,2015).

    Camouflage is likely the main defense mechanism for turtles,as they are slow moving and relatively easy to catch (Hallet al.,2017).Thus,camouflage is an important anti-predation mechanism that improves their fitness.Some tortoise species have been described as having a likeness to stones (Willemsen and Hailey,2003),and evidence suggests that desert tortoises(Gopherus agassizii) select habitats that contain stones similar in size to their own body size,which can reduce their detectability(Nafuset al.,2015).There have been studies of color variation induced in turtles based on their habitats (McGaugh,2008;Roweet al.,2014).However,few studies have explored color and shape similarities between turtles and habitats,or their models(see Xiaoet al.,2016,2021).Moreover,these studies examined the animal as a whole rather than examining whether certain body parts of the animal impact camouflage.

    In our long-term field studies on Indochinese box turtles(C.galbinifrons) and keeled box turtles (C.mouhotii) (Buet al.,2020;Xiaoet al.,2017),we discovered that the carapace roof of the former exhibits a bamboo-leaved pattern,specifically two yellow stripes (Figure 1A),while the carapace roof of the latter exhibits a broad-leaved pattern,created by two keels (Figure 1B).These patterns facilitate disruptive camouflage in their preferred substrates (Buet al.,2020).Both box turtle species bury their bodies in leaf litter when they are still,leaving the middle stripe exposed,which appears to match the surrounding substrate (Figure 2).Both species face predators in the wild,mainly mammals,such as the masked palm civet (Paguma larvata) and wild boar (Sus scrofa) (Xiaoet al.,2020).

    Figure 1 The carapace of an Indochinese box turtle (left) and a keeled box turtle (right).A indicates the yellow stripe of the Indochinese box turtle,and B is the middle stripe of the keeled box turtle.

    Figure 2 Both turtle species bury themselves in leaf litter,leaving the middle stripe exposed.Arrows indicate Indochinese box turtle(Cuora galbinifrons) (A) and keeled box turtle (Cuora mouhotii) (B).

    Figure 3 Four kinds of backgrounds: A is an example of a broad-leaves substrate,B is a bare ground substrate,C is a bamboo-leaves substrate,and D is a grass substrate.

    In the present study,we predicted that the middle stripes on the carapace roof of these twoCuoraspecies had evolved to match their preferred habitat,masquerading as leaves and serving an anti-predation purpose.To the best of our knowledge,this study is the first to focus on the anti-predation benefits associated with partial stripe color and shape variation in box turtles.

    2.Materials and Methods

    2.1.Ethics statementFieldwork was carried out in strict accordance with the guidelines of the Animal Research Ethics Committee of Hainan Provincial Education Centre for Ecology and Environment,Hainan Normal University(HNECEE-2019-006),which conforms to the Law of the People’s Republic of China.No turtles were euthanized for this study,and none were injured while in the traps.All turtles were released in the original capture area at the end of the experiment.

    2.2.Microhabitat selection,photography,and calibrationThis study was conducted from September to December 2019.Field work was conducted in a rainforest in the Diaoluoshan National Nature Reserve (18°43′53′N,109°52′10′E)in Hainan Province,People’s Republic of China.In total,12 adult Indochinese box turtles (carapace length ≥120 mm;seven females and five males) and 12 adult keeled box turtles(carapace length ≥140 mm;eight females and four males)from the reserve were used for this study.The turtles did not exhibit obvious sexual dimorphism in terms of their carapace color.The preferred microhabitats of the keeled box turtle were broad leaves (Figure 3A) and bare ground (Figure 3B),and the preferred microhabitats of the Indochinese box turtle were broad leaves (Figure 3A) and bamboo leaves (Figure 3C),as determined using radio tracking data that recorded the turtles’ movements from April 2015 to February 2016 (for detailed methodology see Xiaoet al.,2017).Grass microhabitats were not favored by either species and served as the control(Figure 3D).Turtles and substrates were photographed using a Canon EOS 550D camera with a Canon lens (EF-S24-70 mm f/2.8,8.2 megapixels).Photography in the field took place between 9:00 a.m.and 4:00 p.m.,when natural light allowed turtles to be seen.A black umbrella was used to counter the direction of light entry and ensure uniform illumination of the photos.A tripod was used to ensure that the lens was perpendicular and placed at the same distance from the substrates,so that a constant image scale could be maintained.We collected 20 images of each substrate type and 24 images of turtles,totaling 104 images.

    Images were saved in an uncompressed RAW format(Stevenset al.,2007) for subsequent color measurements and analyses.The images were calibrated using the “Generate multispectral image” function in ImageJ 1.53e (Wayne Rasband and contributors,National Institutes of Health,USA)(Troscianko and Stevens,2015).The images were ordered linearly based on radiance and standardized to control for differing light conditions by using an 18% standard gray card (Mennon,China,14.5 cm×9.5 cm) (Stevenset al.,2007;Troscianko and Stevens,2015).

    2.3.Color measurement and calculationStandardized images were used to quantify the color contrast between the turtles’ carapace and the substrates,following previously published protocols for images and survey methods (Buet al.,2020).A model of human discrimination was produced using MICA toolbox in ImageJ 1.53e (Wayne Rasband and contributors,National Institutes of Health,USA) (Troscianko and Stevens,2015;Priceet al.,2019;van den Berget al.,2020).As visual searching is an important method that humans use to find turtles (Nafuset al.,2015;Xiaoet al.,2016),we used human eyesight to evaluate whether color differences are distinguishable.According to the RNL model (Receptor Noise Limited Model) (Vorobyev and Osorio,1998),the chromatic just-noticeable difference (CJND) and luminance just-noticeable difference (LJND) determine whether two objects can be differentiated,with increasing values suggesting decreased color similarity.In the present study,we evaluated the color contrast between both species and their substrates by eye.We used the elm 52-mm human D65 calibration profile as a cone model.We compared the entire carapace,the middle stripe,the side stripes(Figure 4),and five randomly selected areas of each substrate with the substrate color.In addition,we randomly selected three to five leaves from each substrate to represent the leaf color.

    Figure 4 Color and structure segmentation diagram for a Cuora galbinifrons individual,with a carapace length of 176.64 mm,including CGM=middle stripe and CGS=side stripe,and a Cuora mouhotii individual,with a carapace length of 161.78 mm,with CMM=middle stripe and CMS=side stripe.

    We converted the standard multispectral image to a cone catch,used the human visual system in sunlight (D65),and set the Weber coefficient as 0.02 (Wyszecki and Stiles,1982)to analyze the chromaticity and luminance of each image(Troscianko and Stevens,2015).The luminance channel was set to “l(fā)um” for the human vision model.We used pixels with a starting size of 2 and an ending size of 512.At each step,the pixels were multiplied by 1.414,to represent exponential growth.Luminance measurements ranged from 0 to 65,535 (Nokelainenet al.,2020).These calculations were performed by a third party who was unaware of the aim of the study.The values of CJND ranged from three to eight,depending on the image content and on each individual subject (Linet al.,2015).

    2.4.Shape measurement and similarity calculationsStandardized images were imported into ImageJ 1.53e and converted into TIFF format.Scales with actual measurement values for area (A) and perimeter (P) were selected.In addition,four further descriptors were used: aspect ratio (AR),solidity (S),circularity (C),and roundness (R).These six shape descriptors were used to quantify the shapes of turtles’ carapaces and the leaves in the substrates.

    According to the similarity algorithm (Samalet al.,2004),descriptor similarity is obtained by standardizing the differences between the prey and the model,as follows:

    where sim (X,Y) is the similarity between objectsXandY;dis(X,Y) represents the Euclidean distance between objectsXandY;andUis the maximum distance between the corresponding features of the two elements.

    The weighted average of similarity between shape descriptorsXandYwas calculated as follows:

    The shape similarity between turtles’ carapace and substrate leaves was then calculated based on Equations 2 and 3,withq=6 representing the six shape descriptors.The values of shape similarity ranged from 0-1,with increasing values equating to higher misidentification rates by human predators (Xiaoet al.,2021).We calculated shape similarity for the entire carapace,the middle stripe,and the side stripes (Figure 4).

    2.5.Statistical analysisAll statistical analyses were performed using SPSS Statistics for Windows,version 18.0 (SPSS Inc.,Chicago,IL,USA),and all data are expressed as mean ± SE.A Kolmogorov-Smirnov test was used to test the normality of CJND,LJND,and the shape similarity data before analysis.Differences in CJND,LJND,and shape between each species,and similarity differences between each substrate type,were analyzed using the independent samples t-test when data were normally distributed;the rest of the data were analyzed using Mann-Whitney U-tests.The significance level was set atP<0.05.

    3.Results

    3.1.Color contrast between turtles and substratesThe CJND of the middle stripe and entire carapace ofC.galbinifronsindividuals were significantly lower in the turtles’ preferred substrates (broad leaves and bamboo leaves) than in the grass substrate.Their side stripe had the lowest contrast with the bamboo-leaves substrate and contrasted more with the broadleaves substrate than with the grass substrate.The CJND of the entire carapace ofC.mouhotiiindividuals was higher on bare ground than on the grass substrate;however,it did not differ significantly between the grass and broad-leaves substrates.The CJND of their middle stripe was significantly higher in their preferred substrates than in the grass substrate,whereas the CJND of their side stripe was significantly lower in their preferred substrates than in the grass substrate (Figure 5,Tables 1-2).

    Table 1 Chromatic just-noticeable difference (CJND) and luminance just-noticeable difference (LJND) between both turtle species and the substrates.

    Table 2 Differences in chromatic just-noticeable difference (CJND) and luminance just-noticeable difference (LJND) between stripes of both turtle species and substrates.

    Figure 5 Vision modeling results of chromatic and luminance contrasts.The top panel shows the chromatic contrast values and the bottom panel shows the luminance contrast values.Different lowercase letters above the columns represent significant differences between each species’ carapace pattern on different substrates (t-tests or Mann-Whitney U-tests;P<0.05),with letter “o” indicating outliers in the data.

    The LJND of the middle stripe,side stripe,and entire carapace ofC.galbinifronsindividuals had the lowest contrast with the bamboo-leaves substrate;however,the LJND of their side stripe was higher in the broad-leaves substrate than it was in the grass substrate.The LJND of the middle stripe and the entire carapace ofC.mouhotiiindividuals was significantly higher in their preferred substrates than in the grass substrate.However,the LJND of their side stripe was lowest in the broadleaves substrate and greater on bare ground than in the grass substrate (Figure 5,Tables 1-2).

    3.2.Shape similarity between turtles and leavesThe entire carapace was the body part that had the lowest similarity between its patterns and the leaves from the three substrate types for both species.ForC.galbinifrons, the middle stripe had the highest similarity with leaves from the broad-leaves substrate,whereas the side stripe had the highest similarity with leaves from the bamboo-leaves substrate.ForC.mouhotii,all carapace parts had the highest shape similarity with leaves from the broad-leaves substrate (Figure 6,Tables 3-4).

    Figure 6 Shape similarity between each species and the leaves in the substrates.Different lowercase letters above the columns represent significant differences between each species’ carapace pattern on different substrates (t-tests or Mann-Whitney U-tests;P<0.05),with hollow cycle “o” indicating outliers in the data.

    Table 3 Average shape similarity between both turtle species and leaves in the substrates.

    Table 4 Shape similarity between stripes of both turtle species and leaves in different substrates.

    3.3.Color contrast between the contrast stripe of both turtle species and leavesThe entire carapace of each turtle species was the body part with the lowest similarity in shape to the leaves of each substrate,meaning that the shape of the entire carapace differs greatly from that of the leaves.Moreover,the carapaces of both species were divided by color or structure,making it an unsuitable feature to analyze color similarity as a whole.It was thus unnecessary to analyze any color similarity between the turtles’ entire carapace and the leaves.

    The side stripe ofC.galbinifronshad a lower CJND with leaves from the bamboo-leaves substrate than with leaves from the broad-leaves and grass substrates.Furthermore,there was no significant difference between the CJND of its side stripe with the grass substrate and that of the broad-leaves substrate.ForC.mouhotii,the CJND between its middle stripe and leaves from its preferred substrates was higher than that between its middle stripe and the grass substrate.

    ForC.galbinifrons,there were no significant differences in LJND between its side stripes and the leaves from grass and broad-leaves substrates;however,the highest LJND value was observed between its side stripes and leaves from the bambooleaves substrate.ForC.mouhotii,the highest LJND occurred between its middle stripe and leaves from the broad-leaves substrate (Figure 7,Tables 5-6).

    Table 6 Differences in chromatic just-noticeable difference (CJND) and luminance just-noticeable difference (LJND) between contrast stripes in each species and leaves in different substrates.

    Figure 7 Vision modeling results of chromatic and luminance contrast between each species’ stripe and leaves in the substrates.The top panel shows the chromatic just-noticeable difference (CJND) and luminance just-noticeable difference (LJND) values between the side stripe of C.galbinifrons and leaves.The bottom panel shows the CJND and LJND values between the middle stripe of C.mouhotii and leaves.Different lowercase letters above the columns represent significant differences between each species’ carapace pattern on different substrates (t-tests or Mann-Whitney U-tests;P<0.05),with hollow cycle “o” indicating outliers in the data.

    Table 5 Chromatic just-noticeable difference (CJND) and luminance just-noticeable difference (LJND) between both species’ contrast stripes and leaves in the substrate.

    4.Discussion

    We assessed the camouflage abilities of twoCuoraspecies in broad-leaves,bamboo-leaves,bare ground,and grass substrates;in all substrates,the CJND was within the human visual discrimination threshold.The color contrast analysis showed that the side stripe ofC.mouhotiiindividuals on their preferred substrates had the lowest CJND and LJND,whereas their middle stripe had the highest CJND and LJND.Conversely,the middle stripe ofC.galbinifronsindividuals on their preferred substrates had the lowest CJND and LJND,whereas their side stripe had the highest CJND and LJND.LJND is not only affected by color but also by the distance from the observer.The part closer to the observer is brighter,while the part further away is darker (Nakayama and Silverman,1986),resulting in a greater LJND in the stripes in different planes and the substrates.The high contrast between the middle and side stripes of each species also indicates that the partial stripes are highlighted in the substrate,which increases the disruptive camouflage for each species in their preferred substrate (Buet al.,2020).

    Our previous long-term studies found that bothCuoraspecies prefer deciduous leaf substrates (Lian,2009;Wang,2007;Xiaoet al.,2017) and habitually semi-cover themselves in leaf litter.The middle stripe ofC.galbinifronsmatched the preferred substrates;however,the greater color difference observed between the middle stripes ofC.mouhotiiand the substrate may be because they are not in the same horizontal plane.When they semi-cover themselves,the middle stripes are in the same plane as the substrate,and the difference is expected to be much smaller.Both box turtle species adopt this strategy of covering themselves with leaves to better match their preferred substrate.Previous studies have demonstrated that some birds,crabs,and insects that cover themselves with decorations can reduce their detectability by predators (Holvecket al., 2017;Ruxton and Stevens,2015).This suggests that potential predators,such as humans,cannot discriminate visually between certain species and the substrate within which they are camouflaged.This strategy adds more noise to object discrimination at an early stage (Stevens and Cuthill,2006) and makes it more difficult for a predator to group the same features together visually(Espinosa and Cuthill,2014;Farmer and Taylor,1980).Our results indicate that both box turtle species may adopt a partial background matching camouflage strategy that reduces the contrast in their predator’s visual field,and they achieve this by semi-covering themselves in leaf litter.

    The middle stripe on the carapace ofC.mouhotiiand the side stripe on the carapace ofC.galbinifronsdid not match their preferred substrates.A high-contrast pattern that does not match the background can impose a high survival cost (Cott,1940).In visual search experiments with specific targets,humans quickly learned that an unusual color is a clue that their target is present (Fraseret al.,2007).In many cases,the patterns on animals appear to be stand out from their background,which may make them vulnerable to predation (Cott,1940;Stevens and Merilaita,2011).However,in some cases a predator may detect prominent features of a prey individual but does not recognize them as part of an object that is worth examining(Cott,1940),an effect that has been demonstrated in humans through eye-tracking experiments (Websteret al.,2013).The middle stripe ofC.mouhotiiand the side stripe ofC.galbinifronsmay be interpreted by predators as leaves rather than as recognizable parts of turtles.Studies have shown that internal contrast markers can in fact provide protection,as predators treat them as separate objects by dividing the entire object into visually disconnected parts (Cuthillet al.,2005;Osorio and Srinivasan,1991;Schaefer and Stobbe,2006;Stevenset al.,2009).

    The middle stripe of bothCuoraspecies shared the highest shape and color similarity levels with leaves from the broadleaves substrate,whereas the side stripe ofC.galbinifronshad the highest shape similarity with the bamboo-leaves substrate.When a predator has a short time frame within which to make a decision,the similarity between the species’ patterns and the background can provide protection (Hallet al.,2017).Several studies have demonstrated that patterned objects on a patterned background result in the slowest identification by predators.When objects are very similar in appearance,a predator’s tracking performance is impeded (Feria,2012;Hallet al.,2017;Howe and Holcombe,2012).The most promising evidence comes from a study on the four-eyed turtle (Sacalia quadriocellata),which showed that higher shape similarity resulted in a higher recognition error rate,indicating more effective camouflage (Xiaoet al.,2021).Interestingly,the middle stripe ofC.mouhotiihad greater chromatic contrast with their preferred substrates than with the grass substrate,meaning that the middle stripe in the preferred substrate can be easily detected by a predator.Despite contrasting in color with their preferred substrates,the middle stripe ofC.mouhotiiand the side stripe ofC.galbinifronsdid have a similar shape to that of the leaves.Previous studies have found that masquerading animals are usually similar in size,color,and shape to their model objects or species (Karpestamet al.,2014;Panettaet al.,2017;Skelhornet al.,2010b;Suzuki and Sakurai,2015).Furthermore,they frequently select the appropriate microhabitats to increase their similarity to the model (Nafuset al.,2015;Skelhorn and Ruxton,2013;Xiaoet al.,2021).For example,the caterpillars of the early thorn moth (Selenia dentaria) masquerade better when the density of a similar model increases (Skelhornet al.,2011).The common potoo bird selects perches on tree stumps that will effectively enhance their masquerading effect (Cestariet al.,2018).We believe that bothCuoraspecies adopt a partial masquerading strategy,rather than using their entire carapace as a means of camouflage.This allows them to increase their masquerading efficiency in varied substrates.

    Our results showed that area accounted for the largest amount of similarity;however,perimeter,AR,S,C,and R(except for the roundness of the side stripe forC.galbinifrons)had high similarity among the four substrate types.Shape and size also have an important influence on the efficiency of camouflage through masquerading.Chiao and Hanlon(2001) found that cuttlefish (Sepia pharaonis) adjust their body markings based on the area of the objects against which they need to camouflage.Skelhorn and Ruxton (2013) found that the size of the moth larvae determines the efficiency of masquerading;however,Suzuki and Sakurai (2015) confirmed that,despite having the same size and area,caterpillars can affect their masquerading efficiency by changing their shape.

    In the present study,area,perimeter,and the other four shape descriptors all had high similarity across habitats.This suggests that the similarity in shape between the turtle species’contrast stripes and the leaves of the habitat types is also a crucial aspect of their masquerading.Furthermore,human trichromatic vision is more sensitive to visual signals than the dichromatic vision of some other mammals (Caveset al.,2018;Trosciankoet al.,2017).Therefore,both turtle species would be camouflaged against the vision of these non-human mammals due to their shape similarity.

    In conclusion,this is the first study to demonstrate that partial masquerading and background matching have a significant impact on turtles’ camouflage efficiency and habitat choice.C.galbinifronsandC.mouhotiiboth exhibited two types of camouflage strategies in their preferred substrates;partial masquerading was apparent when the entire carapace was exposed,and background matching was evident when the turtles were semi-covered in leaf litter.This study introduces a novel approach to camouflage studies that focuses on the influence of shape and color of parts of the body to reduce detectability or recognition.This study evaluated the camouflage effect for human vision,but did not analyze its effect on the turtles’ actual predators in the wild.Future research should explore the effects of turtles’ camouflage strategies on real predators in the wild.

    AcknowledgmentsWe are grateful to Ling WANG,Tingting HUANG,and Zihao YE for assistance with image analysis.We also thank the Diaoluoshan National Nature Reserve for access to the field site and for providing assistance where necessary.Funding was provided by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (31772486) and the Hainan Provincial Natural Science Foundation of China(319MS047 and 320CXTD437).

    免费电影在线观看免费观看| 亚洲国产成人一精品久久久| 精品一区二区免费观看| 成人欧美大片| 国产老妇伦熟女老妇高清| 卡戴珊不雅视频在线播放| 亚洲美女视频黄频| 中文字幕av成人在线电影| av福利片在线观看| 亚洲在久久综合| 一级毛片aaaaaa免费看小| 久久精品久久久久久久性| 嫩草影院入口| 午夜免费激情av| 国产美女午夜福利| 久久草成人影院| 在线 av 中文字幕| 草草在线视频免费看| 日日啪夜夜撸| 欧美日本视频| 亚洲精品456在线播放app| 欧美日韩国产mv在线观看视频 | 亚洲av电影在线观看一区二区三区 | 精品欧美国产一区二区三| 欧美一区二区亚洲| 高清日韩中文字幕在线| 国产精品国产三级专区第一集| 插逼视频在线观看| 免费人成在线观看视频色| av线在线观看网站| 又爽又黄无遮挡网站| 熟女人妻精品中文字幕| 高清欧美精品videossex| 69av精品久久久久久| 亚洲国产欧美人成| freevideosex欧美| 欧美精品一区二区大全| 岛国毛片在线播放| 亚洲在久久综合| a级一级毛片免费在线观看| 欧美成人午夜免费资源| 国产精品一二三区在线看| 人妻少妇偷人精品九色| 日本一二三区视频观看| 欧美成人a在线观看| 欧美日韩视频高清一区二区三区二| 一边亲一边摸免费视频| av在线蜜桃| 日本熟妇午夜| 亚洲精品456在线播放app| 好男人在线观看高清免费视频| 国产综合精华液| 人人妻人人澡欧美一区二区| 午夜免费男女啪啪视频观看| 欧美一级a爱片免费观看看| 啦啦啦啦在线视频资源| 日本欧美国产在线视频| 久久午夜福利片| 一二三四中文在线观看免费高清| 青春草亚洲视频在线观看| 精品久久久久久久久av| 中文精品一卡2卡3卡4更新| 国产男人的电影天堂91| 高清日韩中文字幕在线| 26uuu在线亚洲综合色| 午夜福利高清视频| 欧美变态另类bdsm刘玥| 国产一区有黄有色的免费视频 | 久久久精品94久久精品| 国产不卡一卡二| 一区二区三区高清视频在线| 亚洲在久久综合| 成人一区二区视频在线观看| 亚洲av免费高清在线观看| 免费观看无遮挡的男女| 久久久久久国产a免费观看| 欧美性猛交╳xxx乱大交人| 精品久久国产蜜桃| 嫩草影院精品99| 一级片'在线观看视频| 91av网一区二区| 亚洲精品日韩在线中文字幕| ponron亚洲| 91久久精品电影网| 国产精品久久久久久久电影| 精品少妇黑人巨大在线播放| 久久午夜福利片| 欧美zozozo另类| 国精品久久久久久国模美| 一级毛片久久久久久久久女| 国产亚洲最大av| 丰满乱子伦码专区| 国产黄a三级三级三级人| 免费观看a级毛片全部| 亚洲欧美一区二区三区黑人 | 91久久精品国产一区二区三区| 一边亲一边摸免费视频| 日韩av免费高清视频| 麻豆国产97在线/欧美| 一级片'在线观看视频| 欧美另类一区| 一边亲一边摸免费视频| 99九九线精品视频在线观看视频| 日本av手机在线免费观看| 婷婷六月久久综合丁香| 美女大奶头视频| 免费观看av网站的网址| 亚洲精品乱码久久久v下载方式| 尤物成人国产欧美一区二区三区| 少妇熟女欧美另类| 18禁裸乳无遮挡免费网站照片| 国产精品一区www在线观看| 欧美日韩视频高清一区二区三区二| av国产久精品久网站免费入址| 国产精品爽爽va在线观看网站| 亚洲内射少妇av| 色综合站精品国产| 久久99热这里只频精品6学生| 神马国产精品三级电影在线观看| 国产黄色免费在线视频| 爱豆传媒免费全集在线观看| 国产黄色小视频在线观看| 一区二区三区免费毛片| 一二三四中文在线观看免费高清| 欧美xxxx黑人xx丫x性爽| 亚洲高清免费不卡视频| 国精品久久久久久国模美| 青春草亚洲视频在线观看| 国产一区二区亚洲精品在线观看| av免费在线看不卡| 婷婷六月久久综合丁香| 3wmmmm亚洲av在线观看| 黄片无遮挡物在线观看| 免费看a级黄色片| 少妇丰满av| 人妻少妇偷人精品九色| 乱人视频在线观看| 听说在线观看完整版免费高清| av一本久久久久| 97在线视频观看| 超碰av人人做人人爽久久| 亚洲国产精品成人综合色| av国产久精品久网站免费入址| 午夜精品在线福利| 国产黄色视频一区二区在线观看| 51国产日韩欧美| 青春草国产在线视频| 亚洲一区高清亚洲精品| 亚洲av电影不卡..在线观看| 亚洲不卡免费看| 乱码一卡2卡4卡精品| 成年女人在线观看亚洲视频 | 午夜激情福利司机影院| 麻豆乱淫一区二区| 亚洲精品国产av蜜桃| 久久久欧美国产精品| 国产一区二区三区综合在线观看 | 国产综合懂色| 精品亚洲乱码少妇综合久久| 日韩欧美一区视频在线观看 | 大又大粗又爽又黄少妇毛片口| 伊人久久精品亚洲午夜| 日韩av不卡免费在线播放| 天天一区二区日本电影三级| 久久99精品国语久久久| 欧美日韩综合久久久久久| 搞女人的毛片| 亚洲性久久影院| 精品国内亚洲2022精品成人| 免费看不卡的av| 国产免费又黄又爽又色| 久久亚洲国产成人精品v| 黄色配什么色好看| 亚洲欧美精品自产自拍| 欧美精品一区二区大全| 精品人妻视频免费看| 一级毛片 在线播放| 日韩欧美三级三区| 乱人视频在线观看| 国产片特级美女逼逼视频| 99热全是精品| 精品人妻熟女av久视频| 最近的中文字幕免费完整| 秋霞在线观看毛片| 国产黄色视频一区二区在线观看| 乱系列少妇在线播放| 久99久视频精品免费| 成年人午夜在线观看视频 | 久久国内精品自在自线图片| 国产亚洲精品久久久com| 五月天丁香电影| 天堂俺去俺来也www色官网 | 亚洲成人一二三区av| 又黄又爽又刺激的免费视频.| 秋霞伦理黄片| 日韩国内少妇激情av| 精品少妇黑人巨大在线播放| 日本熟妇午夜| 国产高清不卡午夜福利| 国产精品综合久久久久久久免费| 国产黄频视频在线观看| 亚洲欧美成人综合另类久久久| 亚洲国产欧美在线一区| 亚洲av一区综合| 欧美激情在线99| 亚洲综合精品二区| 非洲黑人性xxxx精品又粗又长| 18禁动态无遮挡网站| 高清在线视频一区二区三区| 欧美高清成人免费视频www| 免费大片黄手机在线观看| 永久网站在线| 亚洲精品成人久久久久久| 最近最新中文字幕免费大全7| 国产精品精品国产色婷婷| 男插女下体视频免费在线播放| 青春草国产在线视频| 午夜久久久久精精品| 国产伦在线观看视频一区| 午夜福利在线观看免费完整高清在| 尾随美女入室| 久久久久久久久久久免费av| 看免费成人av毛片| 久久精品人妻少妇| 国产极品天堂在线| 色吧在线观看| 青青草视频在线视频观看| 成人特级av手机在线观看| 亚洲国产欧美人成| av国产久精品久网站免费入址| 国产精品久久久久久av不卡| 亚洲欧美一区二区三区国产| 亚洲成人一二三区av| 日日啪夜夜爽| 麻豆久久精品国产亚洲av| av国产免费在线观看| 国产一区二区亚洲精品在线观看| 久久精品久久精品一区二区三区| 亚洲一区高清亚洲精品| 国产伦在线观看视频一区| av免费观看日本| 亚洲熟妇中文字幕五十中出| 午夜福利视频精品| 久99久视频精品免费| 乱码一卡2卡4卡精品| 国产在线男女| 欧美最新免费一区二区三区| 边亲边吃奶的免费视频| 亚洲国产精品sss在线观看| 亚洲国产精品专区欧美| 男女国产视频网站| 午夜福利在线观看吧| 99热6这里只有精品| 国产精品久久久久久精品电影小说 | 日韩欧美精品免费久久| 99热6这里只有精品| av线在线观看网站| 亚洲性久久影院| 久久热精品热| 九九久久精品国产亚洲av麻豆| 亚洲av一区综合| 晚上一个人看的免费电影| 亚洲,欧美,日韩| 亚洲精品日韩在线中文字幕| 亚洲伊人久久精品综合| 午夜亚洲福利在线播放| av女优亚洲男人天堂| 中文在线观看免费www的网站| 亚洲欧美成人精品一区二区| 人体艺术视频欧美日本| 哪个播放器可以免费观看大片| 日本免费a在线| 三级毛片av免费| 久久精品国产亚洲av天美| 亚洲精华国产精华液的使用体验| 永久网站在线| 一本—道久久a久久精品蜜桃钙片 精品乱码久久久久久99久播 | 大香蕉久久网| 2022亚洲国产成人精品| 国产成人精品婷婷| 六月丁香七月| 晚上一个人看的免费电影| 久久精品国产亚洲av涩爱| 日日啪夜夜撸| 国产亚洲最大av| 人妻系列 视频| 精品久久久久久久人妻蜜臀av| av网站免费在线观看视频 | 国产黄色小视频在线观看| 国产精品女同一区二区软件| 午夜精品国产一区二区电影 | 极品教师在线视频| 直男gayav资源| 国产探花极品一区二区| 身体一侧抽搐| 国产极品天堂在线| av一本久久久久| 女人十人毛片免费观看3o分钟| 精品一区二区三区视频在线| 亚洲精品456在线播放app| av专区在线播放| 永久网站在线| 国产成人免费观看mmmm| 舔av片在线| 97精品久久久久久久久久精品| 国产成人a区在线观看| 一级毛片黄色毛片免费观看视频| 草草在线视频免费看| 精品午夜福利在线看| 国产精品久久视频播放| 真实男女啪啪啪动态图| 久久热精品热| 日韩成人av中文字幕在线观看| 国产亚洲av嫩草精品影院| 啦啦啦啦在线视频资源| 国产激情偷乱视频一区二区| 永久网站在线| 亚洲av男天堂| 少妇猛男粗大的猛烈进出视频 | 午夜免费激情av| 特大巨黑吊av在线直播| 国产又色又爽无遮挡免| 最近中文字幕2019免费版| 26uuu在线亚洲综合色| 欧美日韩视频高清一区二区三区二| 日韩 亚洲 欧美在线| 天堂网av新在线| 美女脱内裤让男人舔精品视频| 在线观看一区二区三区| 18禁在线播放成人免费| 国产美女午夜福利| 国产色婷婷99| 午夜福利网站1000一区二区三区| 亚洲精品乱久久久久久| 六月丁香七月| 亚洲av在线观看美女高潮| 三级经典国产精品| 99热网站在线观看| 18禁裸乳无遮挡免费网站照片| 亚洲天堂国产精品一区在线| 亚洲成色77777| 菩萨蛮人人尽说江南好唐韦庄| 色播亚洲综合网| 精品久久久久久成人av| 成年女人看的毛片在线观看| 亚洲一级一片aⅴ在线观看| 女人十人毛片免费观看3o分钟| 欧美3d第一页| 国产伦在线观看视频一区| 亚洲欧美日韩无卡精品| 久久综合国产亚洲精品| 欧美丝袜亚洲另类| 老女人水多毛片| 国产老妇女一区| 在线免费观看的www视频| 日日摸夜夜添夜夜爱| 搡老乐熟女国产| 女人被狂操c到高潮| 欧美一级a爱片免费观看看| 婷婷色麻豆天堂久久| 亚洲熟妇中文字幕五十中出| 亚洲aⅴ乱码一区二区在线播放| 免费看av在线观看网站| 免费观看av网站的网址| 哪个播放器可以免费观看大片| 国产黄a三级三级三级人| 大香蕉97超碰在线| 精品久久久久久成人av| 美女脱内裤让男人舔精品视频| 欧美日韩精品成人综合77777| 亚洲精品影视一区二区三区av| 丝瓜视频免费看黄片| 看十八女毛片水多多多| 国产成人91sexporn| 一本久久精品| 久久久欧美国产精品| 国产午夜精品久久久久久一区二区三区| av又黄又爽大尺度在线免费看| 男人和女人高潮做爰伦理| 午夜视频国产福利| 深夜a级毛片| 欧美激情在线99| 又爽又黄a免费视频| 麻豆久久精品国产亚洲av| 超碰av人人做人人爽久久| 亚洲在线观看片| 少妇丰满av| 色视频www国产| 黄色日韩在线| 色吧在线观看| 国产精品嫩草影院av在线观看| 极品少妇高潮喷水抽搐| 日本黄大片高清| 亚洲精品国产成人久久av| 男人狂女人下面高潮的视频| 成人高潮视频无遮挡免费网站| 男人舔女人下体高潮全视频| 毛片一级片免费看久久久久| 日日啪夜夜爽| 精品一区二区三区人妻视频| 久久精品综合一区二区三区| 青春草国产在线视频| 日韩国内少妇激情av| 日本wwww免费看| 亚洲四区av| 亚洲人成网站在线观看播放| 久久国产乱子免费精品| 亚洲精品国产av成人精品| 久久精品熟女亚洲av麻豆精品 | 婷婷六月久久综合丁香| 国产淫片久久久久久久久| 亚洲无线观看免费| 亚洲欧美成人综合另类久久久| 国产淫片久久久久久久久| 高清视频免费观看一区二区 | 免费在线观看成人毛片| 不卡视频在线观看欧美| 色吧在线观看| 久久精品国产亚洲网站| 国产白丝娇喘喷水9色精品| 美女黄网站色视频| 国产美女午夜福利| 国产成人精品福利久久| 亚洲熟妇中文字幕五十中出| 国产单亲对白刺激| 一级毛片黄色毛片免费观看视频| 国产一区亚洲一区在线观看| 久99久视频精品免费| 伊人久久精品亚洲午夜| 亚洲精品乱久久久久久| 99热这里只有是精品在线观看| 成年版毛片免费区| 麻豆国产97在线/欧美| 99热这里只有是精品在线观看| 黄色配什么色好看| 97超视频在线观看视频| 丰满少妇做爰视频| 看黄色毛片网站| av国产免费在线观看| 大香蕉97超碰在线| 久久久久免费精品人妻一区二区| 美女主播在线视频| 久久国内精品自在自线图片| 国产在线男女| 久久99热这里只有精品18| 最近的中文字幕免费完整| 女人久久www免费人成看片| 色综合站精品国产| 亚洲av不卡在线观看| 你懂的网址亚洲精品在线观看| 久久精品夜夜夜夜夜久久蜜豆| 舔av片在线| 99热这里只有精品一区| 内射极品少妇av片p| 亚洲伊人久久精品综合| 久久精品国产亚洲网站| 全区人妻精品视频| 2021天堂中文幕一二区在线观| 色播亚洲综合网| 亚洲国产色片| 成人综合一区亚洲| 最近最新中文字幕免费大全7| 一级黄片播放器| 不卡视频在线观看欧美| 久久久精品94久久精品| 男人狂女人下面高潮的视频| 欧美不卡视频在线免费观看| 777米奇影视久久| 在线免费观看不下载黄p国产| 九九在线视频观看精品| 少妇的逼好多水| 欧美极品一区二区三区四区| 午夜福利高清视频| 久久韩国三级中文字幕| 欧美变态另类bdsm刘玥| 久久久久久久久大av| 欧美丝袜亚洲另类| 美女脱内裤让男人舔精品视频| 国产精品女同一区二区软件| 精品久久久噜噜| 又大又黄又爽视频免费| 99热全是精品| 亚洲欧美精品自产自拍| 日日摸夜夜添夜夜爱| 国产探花在线观看一区二区| 一级毛片 在线播放| 国产久久久一区二区三区| 精品国产三级普通话版| 女人被狂操c到高潮| 国产精品综合久久久久久久免费| 一个人观看的视频www高清免费观看| 国产午夜精品一二区理论片| 亚洲怡红院男人天堂| 水蜜桃什么品种好| av福利片在线观看| 美女xxoo啪啪120秒动态图| 亚洲精品一区蜜桃| 国产爱豆传媒在线观看| 毛片一级片免费看久久久久| 国产精品人妻久久久久久| 我的女老师完整版在线观看| 韩国高清视频一区二区三区| 免费观看av网站的网址| 两个人视频免费观看高清| 国产激情偷乱视频一区二区| 菩萨蛮人人尽说江南好唐韦庄| 99热这里只有是精品在线观看| 午夜福利网站1000一区二区三区| 少妇裸体淫交视频免费看高清| 欧美一级a爱片免费观看看| 久久精品国产自在天天线| 最近的中文字幕免费完整| 亚洲欧美精品自产自拍| 午夜福利视频精品| 看免费成人av毛片| 天美传媒精品一区二区| 国产精品麻豆人妻色哟哟久久 | 久久久久精品性色| 国产精品久久久久久久电影| 一个人观看的视频www高清免费观看| 美女被艹到高潮喷水动态| 亚洲国产精品专区欧美| 亚洲av不卡在线观看| 只有这里有精品99| 简卡轻食公司| 国产免费福利视频在线观看| 午夜福利在线观看吧| 国产成人精品福利久久| 一级二级三级毛片免费看| 午夜免费观看性视频| 国产国拍精品亚洲av在线观看| 国产色爽女视频免费观看| 亚洲综合色惰| 别揉我奶头 嗯啊视频| 国产黄频视频在线观看| 国产精品嫩草影院av在线观看| av福利片在线观看| 91在线精品国自产拍蜜月| 久久久久久久久中文| 亚洲国产欧美在线一区| 日日啪夜夜爽| 日本wwww免费看| 亚洲久久久久久中文字幕| 2021天堂中文幕一二区在线观| 精品人妻熟女av久视频| 亚洲第一区二区三区不卡| 精品久久久精品久久久| 久久久久久久久久久免费av| 91在线精品国自产拍蜜月| 久久久久久久久久成人| 亚洲最大成人av| 91精品一卡2卡3卡4卡| 欧美另类一区| 国产激情偷乱视频一区二区| 一级片'在线观看视频| 国产日韩欧美在线精品| 日韩欧美三级三区| 精品久久久久久久久久久久久| 日韩不卡一区二区三区视频在线| 国产精品蜜桃在线观看| 哪个播放器可以免费观看大片| 欧美 日韩 精品 国产| 午夜福利在线在线| 在线 av 中文字幕| 成人毛片60女人毛片免费| 国产老妇伦熟女老妇高清| 免费av不卡在线播放| 久久99热这里只频精品6学生| 三级经典国产精品| 一级毛片我不卡| 国产成人精品福利久久| 18禁在线播放成人免费| 日本熟妇午夜| 欧美性猛交╳xxx乱大交人| 成人欧美大片| 国产精品爽爽va在线观看网站| 免费人成在线观看视频色| 在线免费观看的www视频| 国产大屁股一区二区在线视频| 国产一区二区在线观看日韩| 免费看av在线观看网站| 黄片wwwwww| 狠狠精品人妻久久久久久综合| 美女主播在线视频| 中文字幕亚洲精品专区| 日韩伦理黄色片| 高清毛片免费看| 婷婷六月久久综合丁香| 直男gayav资源| 色吧在线观看| 又黄又爽又刺激的免费视频.| 丰满人妻一区二区三区视频av| 观看免费一级毛片| 国产精品人妻久久久影院| 亚洲三级黄色毛片| 精品久久国产蜜桃| 少妇的逼水好多| 噜噜噜噜噜久久久久久91| 久久久久久久久久成人| 91久久精品国产一区二区成人| 最近中文字幕2019免费版| 99久久精品国产国产毛片| 午夜福利在线观看免费完整高清在| 国产精品熟女久久久久浪| ponron亚洲| 人妻系列 视频| 深夜a级毛片| 内地一区二区视频在线| 婷婷六月久久综合丁香| 亚洲精品一区蜜桃| 爱豆传媒免费全集在线观看| 免费少妇av软件| 夫妻午夜视频| 在线播放无遮挡| 一个人免费在线观看电影|